[Thread Prev] | [Thread Next] | [Thread Index] | [Date Prev] | [Date Next] | [Date Index] |
The following announcement is posted at the request of Howard Bloom, whose name no doubt many of you will recognize. Bloom's group's proposal is interesting and general topic of "paleopsychology" is something paleontologists have already given some attention to, albeit under a variety of other names. However, the idea of paleontologists being invited to establish formal links to colleagues in the more traditional behavioral disciplines in this manner is, to my mind, unprecedented. I hope the paleontological community will respond positively to this intriguing initiative. Perhaps someone who works in this are could also take a moment to bring us up-to-date as to where the field of behavioral studies in ancient animals is now and where it might be going. Norm MacLeod ---------- We have started a new discipline--paleo-bio-socio-psychology or paleopsychology for short. The enclosed manifesto was written at the request of the Journal of the Across Species Comparisons and Psychopathology Society. We hope you find it of some interest. Howard Bloom ------------------------ MANIFESTO FOR A NEW PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE by Howard Bloom (member: New York Academy of Sciences, American Association for the Advancement of Science, American Psychological Society, Academy of Political Science, Human Behavior and Evolution Society, European Sociobiological Society) My collaborators and I propose the establishment of a new discipline: paleo-bio-socio-psychology or "paleopsychology" for short. Each of us has already taken initial steps toward creating a corpus of paleopsychological knowledge. We welcome those who would like to join us. Standard paleontology has done a magnificent job of recreating the morphology of creatures ranging from the first life forms 3.85 billion years ago to the early humans of comparatively recent times. In the case of the majority of pre-historic species, however, paleontology has left us with a considerable problem. How did these creatures behave? What, if any, were their social patterns? What cognitive and problem-solving abilities did they possess? What was the bio-evolutionary sequence which led to learning, imitation, herding, information sharing, and to what John Tyler Bonner has called animal culture? Primate fossil evidence has often been looked at with an eye to inferring the origins of campsites, tools, migratory patterns, "mental modules," and some of the subject matter of which paleopsychology is made. Similarly, dinosaur remains have been scrutinized for signs of maternal nurturance and other indicators of social attachment and of the ability to tell one conspecific from another. But what of the social interactions and reactive powers of the earliest bacteria, the first eukaryotes, the recently-discovered Precambrian clams, and the Cambrian profusion of phyletic representatives--from trilobites to eurypterids? What about the first insects of 350 mya--were they initially solitary, as E.O. Wilson and numerous others assume, or were they social, as one of us suspects? Was individuality or sociality the original state of living beings? If the latter, how did the anomaly of solitary existence emerge? If the former, where does sociality begin in the fossil record, and why? The tools with which these questions can be probed are few today, but will surely expand as more minds join the quest. Mass- behavior-specialist Howard Bloom has used data on bacterial social behavior along with fossil evidence to postulate that the cyanobacteria of 3.5 billion years ago were not only extraordinarily social, but that their colonies exhibited what physicist-turned-microbiologist Eshel ben Jacob calls a collective "creative" intelligence. Extrapolating from the work of Sorin Sonea and Maurice Panisset (1983), Bloom has gone on to make the case that the Pre-cambrian system of prokaryotic information exchange was literally worldwide. In addition, Bloom has penned four papers for Germany's Telepolis tracing the history of the cooperative impulse and of cognitive development from the first 10(-32) second of the Big Bang to 35 million b.p. Combined with the data of Ben Jacob and of the University of Chicago's James Shapiro, Bloom's published views call into question fundamental axioms of neo-Darwinist evolutionary theory. Invertebrate zoologist Kerry B. Clark, creator of the definitive teaching CD-ROM Metazoa, has applied the rules of his field to the fossil record, tentatively recreating Cambrian social behavior. Among other things, he hypothesizes that Anomalocaris canadensis swam in feeding herds. "The largest animals in most ecosystems are typically herding herbivores," he notes, "and I see nothing about Anomalocaris that precludes this." Paleontologist Kevin Brett, who spent five years working at the Burgess Shale for the Royal Ontario Museum under the sponsorship of National Geographic Magazine, disagrees about Anomalocaris, but cites evidence that trilobites may well have been sexually dimorphic, and that many trilobites were, in his words, "quite ornate." Brett also points to the well-known observation that, "Trilobites are often found in mass associations of mono-specific gatherings of complete individuals. This suggests mating and/or moulting gatherings such as those observed in modern marine arthropods such as Limulus (Horseshoe crabs). Evidence has been found for multispecific gatherings as well as physical processes such as wave and current transport." From this and the positioning of trilobites in fossil beds, he proposes that trilobite sexual gatherings may not have been entirely promiscuous. Modern "toads," he points out, "will mate with just about anything--so they don't necessarily recognize members of even their own species." Brett suspects that Cambrian arthropods were more discerning. Entomologist Christine Nalepa cites an understudied source of data, trace fossils. From fecal remains in chambers carved in dead Carboniferous tree ferns, she infers that the earliest proto- cockroaches (Cryptocercidae-like insects) may have shown active social behavior 300 million years ago--over 160 million years before even the most extreme dates hypothesized for the emergence of eusociality. As Brett points out, "All animals are social. We have the opportunity to trace the degrees of sociality in the fossil record using burrow and hive traces, mass associations, nests, etc." Adds Clark, "The chemical transmitters in the most advanced organisms have their precursors in the simple biochemically-mediated behavioral responses of bacteria and protists, indicating a continuity of mechanisms between these extremes. "The basic organizational features of the most advanced nervous systems -- ganglionation, condensation of diffuse sensors into discrete organs, and interneuronal processing -- that we associate with intelligent behavior, are expressed in all but the simplest animals, and it is reasonable to look for, and expect, some expression of intelligent behaviors in 'lower' animals. Social behaviors, by assembling superorganisms, facilitate 'emergent properties' that can assemble intelligent behaviors not found in solitary forms, optimizing exploitation of their environments, and may or may not be associated with fossil evidence of the superorganism. The two prime correlates of intelligence, organism size and complexity, can arise both in big, complex individuals and in smaller organisms that communally form large, complex units of biomass. Our knowledge and recognition of such social interactions is still at an early stage." Bloom, Clark, Brett and Nalepa are all members of our group. But we have illustrious forebears. Charles Darwin hinted at a psychology of the creatures which preceded us in his Expression of Emotions in Man and Animals (1872). With Darwin's blessings, George Romanes took the query a step further in his 1884 Mental Evolution In Animals. Lynn Margulis has done a masterful job of reconstructing the lives of what she calls "microbial communities in the Archean and Proterozoic Eons." Margulis credits as other predecessors Schimper (in his work of 1833), Famintzyn (1891), Mereschkovsky (1909), Portier (1918) and Wallin (1927)--all concerned, as is Margulis, with evolutionary cell biology. In addition, B. Moore has worked recently on reconstructing the evolution of imitative learning. Yet the area explored by these pioneers has often been forgotten once the researchers responsible have gone. It is time to end this periodic amnesia. The tools exist. The evidence exists. And the need to know is there. The evolution of behavior, sociality, and the physiology of proto-mentation finally deserve a discipline of their own. If you wish more information on paleopsychology, or would like to join us in our quest, please e-mail or phone: Howard Bloom 705 President Street Brooklyn, NY 11215 phone 718 622 2278 fax 718 398 2551 e-mail howlbloom@aol.com For further data on the participants and a taste of their accomplishments, see: www.bookworld.com/lucifer (re Howard Bloom); http://users.aol.com/kbclark/cambrian and http://users.aol.com/kbclark/metahome (Dr. Kerry B. Clark); www.ualberta.ca/~kbrett/Trilobites.html and www.ualberta.ca/~kbrett/index.html (re Kevin Brett); and Nalepa, Christine (1994), "Nourishment and the Origin of Termite Eusociality," in Nourishment and Evolution in Insect Societies, edited by James H. Hunt and Christine A. Nalepa, 1994, Boulder, Colorado: Westview Press: 57-96. -------------------------------------------- Things are moving so rapidly that despite a month of 7 AM to 4:30 AM workdays, I am having trouble keeping up with them. 1) Telepolis Magazine, a german web publication from the Heise publishing house, has offered to do two things. It is already printing the chapters of my book outlining twelve billion years of paleopsychology, and will do so until that book's completion. It has also made available to our group--now called The International Paleopsychology Project--an "open forum" in which members may advance their ideas. These will be targeted essays by major scientists (of which we have many--there are roughly 40 of us now), each adding a piece to a twelve-billion-year mosaic. 2) My agent, Richard Curtis, president of the Association of Author's Representatives, wants our key members each to conceive a book for the mass audience, a work along the lines of "The Selfish Gene" or "The Lives of a Cell"--presenting important new ideas but in a manner any intelligent person can understand and even relish. I will act as executive editor. Each work must not only surprise with its insights, but must contribute to the mosaic we are creating, one based on a very new scientific worldview. Some members may simply add to the paleopsychology timeline. Others are activists in a secondary goal for which I've been gathering researchers from around the world these last two years--the proposal of a post-neo-Darwinian paradigm, one which does for current evolutionary theory what Relativity did for Newtonianism. The goal is not to negate the highly productive concepts of the past, but to demonstrate the sphere within which they belong, then stretch out to encompass the universe of empirical data which the old ideas cannot embrace. Our first book is likely to be from Eshel Ben Jacob, whose papers in Nature, Physica A, Contemporary Physics and others over the last seven years have given both the evidence and theory for a leap the physicist/microbiologist calls "orthogonal to Darwinism." My own book offering a post neo-Darwinian vision will also be a part of the series. And proposals are now flowing in from other group members. Please increase the rate of that flow if you can. Our agent is chomping at the bit to take a complete package of book proposals to the top execs at the major publishing houses. 3) There is talk--just talk, but serious talk indeed--of an International Paleopsychology Project gathering. We shall see if this comes to fruition. Could anyone who might be able to contribute to the creation of such a thing contact me, and I will put him in touch with the person who is crafting this effort. 4) One of our computer scientists, Alexander Chislenko, is on the verge of doing an independent project at MIT under the aegis of Marvin Minsky which will parallel Howard Bloom's exploration of the history of information pooling, but do so in terms of information theory. Chislenko will then extrapolate from the past and project future possibilities of the collective intelligence through the growth of symbiotic forms of wide-scale human/machine cooperation models and distributed artificial intelligence. The technological part of the project will involve designing cooperative knowledge sharing network as an active intelligent extension to today's World Wide Web. Information on Chislenko's writings and projects is available on-line at 5) Our present e-mail center will gradually become a laboratory for virtual collaboration, more project than chat oriented. But this will take time. Even the best of labs cease their productivity when their lunchrooms are shuttered and informal conversation ends. So we must achieve a balance. I hope this gives you at least an idea of our momentum since we first went public with four scientists on April 18th. As of Sunday April 27 at 4:00 PM, the group organized around paleopsychology consisted of the following members (in the ensuing weeks, 26 more have joined, some of them illustrious in the world of science; they will be announced in further correspondence): The work of Tel-Aviv University's Eshel ben Jacob--a physicist-turned-microbiologist--on bacterial "creative webs" is paradigm breaking. In a paper to be published momentarily by Physica A, "Bacterial Wisdom, Godel's Theorem and Creative Genomic Webs," Dr. Ben Jacob challenges the roots of current neo-Darwinism. Ben Jacob's studies of bacterial colony formation have appeared with great regularity in Contemporary Physics, Nature, Fractals, Physical Review Letters, Physical Review E, etc. In addition he is co-editor of "The Physics of Biological Systems: from molecules to species," Berlin, Springer, 1997. His contribution to this book is entitled "Smart Bacterial Colonies." Ben-Jacob's "Cooperative formation of bacterial colonies" appears in "Bacteria as Multicellular Organisms," edited by J.A. Shapiro and M. Dwarkin, Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1997. Howard Bloom is a member of the New York Academy of Sciences, American Association for the Advancement of Science, American Psychological Society, Academy of Political Science, Human Behavior and Evolution Society, European Sociobiological Society, and has lectured at Wesleyan University, Georgia State University, CUNY, and New York University. As a mass-behavior-specialist, Bloom has used data on bacterial social behavior along with fossil evidence to postulate that the cyanobacteria of 3.5 billion years ago were not only social, but that their colonies exhibited what Eshel ben Jacob calls a collective "creative" intelligence. In addition, Bloom has penned four papers for Germany's Telepolis tracing the history of the cooperative impulse and of cognitive and proto-memetic development from the first 10(-32) second of the Big Bang to 35 mya. Bloom's work calls into question fundamental axioms of neo-Darwinist evolutionary theory. All this is the culmination of a research agenda Mr. Bloom undertook in 1956. Bloom has been aiming at a post-neo-Darwinian synthesis since 1981. More recently, Bloom was the only scientist asked to organize a panel at the 1996 Annual Meeting of the European Sociobiological Society. He has been featured in every edition of "Who's Who in Science and Engineering" since the publication's inception. In 1995, Bloom founded an informal academic circle called "the group selection squad," whose efforts helped re-legitimate the discussion of group selection within the evolutionary community. Mr. Bloom's book, "The Lucifer Principle: a scientific expedition into the forces of history" (New York: Atlantic Monthly Press, 1995) has been called "A revolutionary vision of the relationship between psychology and history" (Elizabeth F. Loftus, Professor of Psychology, University of Washington, author of Memory and Eyewitness Testimony); "a long step forward in the human effort to understand human biology" (Dr. Richard Bergland, M.D., researcher on brain endocrinology, founder of the department of neurosurgery, Sloan/Kettering, author of "The Fabric of Mind"); and "a freshly viable theory of human social evolution" (The Washington Times). Paleontologist Kevin Brett, of the Department of Earth and Atmospheric Sciences, University of Alberta, Canada, spent five years working at the Burgess Shale for the Royal Ontario Museum under the sponsorship of National Geographic Magazine. Brett's experience with trilobites and other Paleozoic fossils both in the field and in the lab is extensive. He is currently engaged in Ph.D. research on Devonian trilobites and maintains the leading trilobite site on the WWW. His fieldwork has covered nearly all of Canada, most of the eastern USA, and Morocco. Brett explains that his "love and experience with trilobites started when I could crawl." He fails to mention that by the time he began his crawling, the trilobites had ended theirs. Invertebrate zoologist Kerry B. Clark, creator of the definitive teaching CD-ROM "Metazoa(tm)," has applied the rules of his field to the fossil record, graphically recreating Paleozoic social behavior. One result has been the visually and informationally stunning CD-Rom "Metazoa," designed for museum and educational use. Dr. Clark is Professor of Biological Sciences at the Florida Institute of Technology. He says, "My research interests include all aspects of the biology of opisthobranch gastropod molluscs (sea slugs); physiological and population ecology; conservation of marine invertebrates; and computer applications in biology." NYU neurophysiologist Edgar E. Coons, leads a "laboratory devoted to studying drive and hedonic mechanisms in the brain and how they interact." Dr. Coons is the discoverer of lateral hypothalamic stimulation's ability to elicit feeding, ameliorate anxiety, and lower pain in rats. He has been published in Science, The Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Brain Research, Behavioral Neuroscience and the Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, among others. The Director of Palo Alto's Institute for the Study of Complex Systems, Dr. Peter Corning is author of "The Synergism Hypothesis" (New York: McGraw Hill, 1983) and the upcoming "Holistic Darwinism" -- now in press at the Journal of Social and Evolutionary Systems. He writes, "I taught in the interdisciplinary Human Biology Program at Stanford for seven years and held a research appointment in Stanford's interdisciplinary Engineering-Economic Systems Department. I also spent two years on an NIMH post-doctoral research fellowship at the Institute for Behavioral Genetics (University of Colorado) and published laboratory experimental work on the genetics of aggression. More recently, I was a fellow at the Collegium Budapest (Institute for Advanced Study) in Hungary. I have also published three books (two on synergy) and, frankly, an uncounted number of articles." The University of Ghent's Koen DePryck is author of:"Knowledge, evolution and paradox: the ontology of language," SUNY Press; of the forthcoming "Possible evolutionary advantages of learning difficulties"; and creator of a larger project called "the archeology of mind". Columbia University's Ralph Holloway, a Fellow of the New York Academy of Sciences and the AAAS, was involved in the landmark work by Marion Diamond et. al. demonstrating the relationship of dendritic branching to stimulus in rats. His major areas of concentration are sexual dimorphism in the human and chimpanzee corpus callosum, and paleoneurology. He says, "I've made many of the brain endocasts of our fossil ancestors from Africa, and have traveled there as well as Indonesia and Europe to endocast the crania....I tend to believe that careful analyses of stone toolmaking is our best bet for probing hominid cognition, a view I've been too rigid to let go since my 1969 'Culture: A Human Domain' paper in Current Anthropology. I also have an interest in aggression, and even edited a book on it in 1974." David Smillie functioned as a developmental psychologist for forty years and spent the last twenty five of those years as a professor at New College, University of South Florida. Since 1993 he has been a visiting professor in the Department of Zoology at Duke University where he has been pursuing a study, begun twenty five years ago, of social evolution. He has published articles on both these fields in a variety of journals and books. -------------------------------------- to join or to obtain further information contact Howard Bloom (member: New York Academy of Sciences, American Association for the Advancement of Science, American Psychological Society, Academy of Political Science, Human Behavior and Evolution Society, European Sociobiological Society) 705 President Street Brooklyn, NY 11215 phone 718 622 2278 fax 718 398 2551 e-mail howlbloom@aol.com for two chapters from The Lucifer Principle: A Scientific Expedition Into the Forces of History, see www.bookworld.com/lucifer The Lucifer Principle:a scientific expedition into the forces of history ___________________________________________________________________ Dr. Norman MacLeod Micropalaeontological Research N.MacLeod@nhm.ac.uk (E-mail) Department of Palaeontology, The Natural History Museum, Cromwell Road, London, SW7 5BD Office Phone: 0171-938-9006 Dept. FAX: 0171-938-9277 E-mail: N.MacLeod@nhm.ac.uk ___________________________________________________________________
Partial index: